london@oups.org.uk

Moves Like Jagger-bot: "We Can Make A Robot Dance, But Can We Really Make It Dance?"

Clara Clein Wolfe • 1 January 2021

Social Robots with Professor Emily Cross: From Social Cognition To Social Robots.

Considering the apocalyptic madness of 2020, one could be forgiven for being surprised that neither the aliens dropped by, nor the robots, led by their self-checkout overlords, began their inevitable uprising. A fear of the unknown and apprehension about the march of technological innovation can elicit a sense of, THEY'RE COMING THEY'RE COMING! However, what is the reality? Is the uprising truly inevitable? Is the robot fear justified?


Following the brilliant March conference, Technology and Psychology, London OUPS hosted Professor Emily S. Cross. Emily is a cognitive neuroscientist jointly based at the University of Glasgow and Macquarie University in Australia, with an interest in how different experiences shape human brains and behaviours. Emily is the principal investigator on the European Research Council Starting Grant, and director of the Social Brain in Action Laboratory at Macquarie University.


In a lively lecture delivered over the internet from Australia, Emily talked about a range of interesting topics including; social cognition, working with artificial agents, the implications of how we perceive artificial agents, and finally, she shared a thought-provoking quote before a brief question and answer session.


Social cognition refers to how people understand the actions and behaviours of others in a social world. Emily referenced how significant social cognition has become owing to the ongoing pandemic and how it has changed societies, such as the increase of screen-mediated communication. The human brain has evolved to make sense of the gap between a person's actions and the perception of those actions in a bidirectional loop. Emily's research investigates how different types of experiences shape perception, for example, dancer Emily is particularly interested in motor learning and expertise. Other research areas include observational learning, the rapidly evolving field of neuroaesthetics, plus work with artificial agents, or robots.


It is remarkable how much working with artificial agents can reveal about human social cognition. A core element of social perception is the theory of 'like me', which refers to the importance placed on similarity, or, "me as a template to understand you" (Meltzoff & Prinz, 2003, Meltzoff, 2007). Emily mentioned evidence which supported this assertion [Meltzoff & Prinz, 2003], and those which suggest greater flexibility in social information processing [Cross et al., 2009, Ramsey & Hamilton, 2010]. This influenced Study 1 [Cross et al., 2012] which looked at perceiving human versus robot form and motion. The results were surprising as they were in contravention to the "like me" hypothesis because the participants showed more reaction to robotic dancing than human dancing. Emily suggested two explanations for this surprising finding: low-level action features or, greater engagement of compensatory top-down modulation. It inspired further research.


People's perception of artificial agents influences their reaction and behaviour, which Emily demonstrated in the lecture using, for example, Fembots. A Fembot, for the uninitiated (or young!) is an ostensibly attractive blonde woman from the 1997 Austin Powers film. Looking at the film still, you either have the stimulus cue of what you see or the knowledge cue of having seen the film and knowing that the negligee-wearing femmes were especially fatale as they are in fact, robots with a pair of guns in their chests: the name is a portmanteau. To manipulate cues, participants in Study 2 [Cross et al., 2016], watched a professional video explaining the history and industry use of motion capture and computer animation technology. In the study, they were then told that the video they were watching was made using either motion capture or computer animation thus manipulating their beliefs. When rating the videos for smoothness and extent to having liked them, the knowledge cues had greater impact than stimulus cues. Emily identified the potential for these findings to influence the design of artificial agents and to manage expectations.


Research suggests that social mimicry, or, automatic imitation, can help develop social bonds [Chartrand & van Baaren, 2009, Heyes, 2011]. Does this social mimicry extend to nonhuman agents and what is the effect of stimulus and knowledge cues to automatic imitation? Study 3 [Klapper, Ramsey, Wigboldus & Cross, 2014] investigated interacting with artificial agents and the social mimicry phenomena. Once again, participants watched the video from Study 2 to manipulate cues. Results indicated that if there are cues to human-ness, participants are more likely to show social mimicry. Emily summarised the robot study findings as indicating that a social cognitive neuroscience-based approach to investigating and improving the human elements of human-nonhuman agent interaction.

Emily introduced us to the 'robotic empire' who are being built and developed to tackle ever more complicated questions. The robots included, Pepper, who is the size of a small human and has humanistic facial features, particularly big eyes. Cosmo, on the other hand, fits in your palm. Nao is a toddler-sized 'workhorse in social robotics', who similarly has a human appearance. Miro is a robot who looks like a dog and who many of us encountered at the Technology and Psychology conference in Professor Tony Prescott's lecture. Emily mentioned that she is currently working with Miro in Australia. Indeed, there is a lot of exciting work with robots being conducted, which Emily referenced, for example, further work on automatic imitation, collaboration, hared representations, empathy for pain after longer-term social interactions, and, synchrony and social reward. It is an exciting and evolving field of study and one that is worth following for the developments.


Emily spoke about several additional studies including interesting research about empathy for pain and neural overlaps [Cross, Riddoch, Pratts, Titone, Chaudhury & Hortensius, 2019]. Does spending time interacting with social robots lead to participants developing more overlap in neural mechanisms when observing a human or robot in pain or pleasure and then vice versa? The hypothesis was that there would be more repetition suppression [reduced neural response observed when stimuli are presented more than once], for agents not emotions, after the socialising intervention. The socialising intervention was a schedule of tasks participants carried out at home with a Cosmo robot. Cosmo can learn things, can recognise faces as well as being programmed to be receptive to cats and dogs. One participant had shared a spectacular picture of Cosmo sat on the dinner table while their pet cat stealthily stared at the unfamiliar (relatively speaking as the at-home element was five days) interloper to their home. Emily gave a brief summary of the complicated results from this study, which included a null result, thus suggesting that social cognition is not so easily challenged by an arguably brief amount of time socialising with a robot. The result may have been disappointing, but it nevertheless contributes to the body of knowledge.

A further study Emily talked about concerned synchrony and social reward. Synchrony refers to simultaneous action, for example, dancing. Synchrony is important in social cognition because when people synchronise, it can lead to increased bonding and engage significant regions in the brain. Participants were in an experiment with Pepper involving them both tracing shapes. Participants also could ask Pepper questions, with the idea that if participants were synchronising with the robot, that they would ask more questions. Results indicated that synchronising with Pepper did not seem to change people's social motivation towards Pepper. An additional null result can seem similarly disappointing however, Emily highlighted that when scientists are conducting studies, the literature search reveals results upon which they [we!] draw upon and so, despite this study having a null result, the next researcher investigating this topic will be able to draw upon both results: the scientific cycle of inquiry.


At the end of the lecture, Emily shared a quote from Erik Sofge: "we're only barely scratching the surface of the brain's social algorithms, which become even more complicated and unpredictable when we interface with technology". This was an interesting quote to end this fascinating lecture because it encapsulated two of the biggest themes of the lecture and concerns of contemporary society. The complexity of the brain is such that there is so much which is yet to be discovered, plus things that we thought could be predicted have ended up not being represented when tested. Alongside this, there is the advancement of technology and the interaction of these factors. Answering one of the last questions, Emily described making increasingly humanoid robots as a 'fool's errand' and said that there are more important considerations than how the robot looks or moves. However, Emily pointed out that the idea of making our robot double is 'too seductive'.


Returning to the self-checkout robot uprising, despite my personal knowledge of the evil within those infuriating hell devices, there is nothing to suggest the Machiavellian machines are mobilising. On the contrary, the social robots covered by Emily's lecture and the Technology and Psychology conference prior, are lovers not fighters. Arguably, the null results from Miro and Pepper studies show humans in a questionable light. Moreover, the social robots are being used with vulnerable groups, such as the elderly [Laban et al., 2020, Paladyn, Riddoch & Cross, under review], and to investigate the development of theory of mind toward a variety of machines [Jastrzab et al. in prep; ongoing]. The fear and apprehension about robots may be a Hollywood-mediated effect. There is also the elephant in the room that behind every increasingly intelligent robot, are the humans who created it. Ultimately, OUPS' David posed the question to leave us all with something to think about: 'we can make a robot dance, but can we really make it dance?'.



A massive thank you to Emily for her wonderful lecture, and to all at LOUPS for ending this year with another fantastic event. 2020 has been bewildering, challenging and, of course, unprecedented. Despite the drastic changes we have all experienced, LOUPS have still hosted fab events and got inspirational and motivational speakers. Roll on 2021!


by Clara Clein Wolfe 31 Mar, 2024
London Open University Psychological Society Conference. 16th March 2024 London School of Economics.
by Clara Clein Wolfe 31 Jan, 2024
LOUPS Criminology Seminar – 11 th November 2023
by Clara Clein Wolfe 26 Nov, 2023
Magic, mishaps, and maybe toilets: "mindreading" to mitigate disaster. Professor Graham Edgar. (University of Gloucester)  LOUPS Pub Social – October 2023.
by David Byrne 26 Sept, 2023
Kris Ambler (BACP): How to become a BACP-registered Counsellor
by Clara Clein Wolfe 27 Apr, 2023
LOUPS Spring Conference 2023: Mental Health – Working in the System.
by Clara Clein Wolfe 24 Oct, 2022
Criminal Minds: aspects of criminal motivation and behaviour
by Clara Clein Wolfe 03 Aug, 2022
The collective appetite for true crime shows no sign of abating. The adage ‘if it bleeds, it leads’ spans media reporting, television tropes, and documentaries. The deviance of humanity is an enduring topic of investigation. This Summer, Frederick Toates and Olga Coschug-Toates released their book, Understanding Sexual Serial Killing. It is a brilliant read which delves into the myriad aspects of sexual serial killing, such as underlying factors, childhood experiences, and the biological, psychological, and social features. The book is divided into three parts: the factors that underlie lust killing, biographical sketches, and a final word. The factors which underlie lust killing, also known as sexual serial killing, span everything from the brain, motivations, and context. The brain is a source of interest in considering human differences and deviations. For example, Fred and Olga demonstrates the value of studying ‘normal’ brains and those which have been stretched beyond normal/adaptive functioning. However, these [predominantly] men need motivation to commit these crimes and the associated activities such as stalking. There is also the relevance of the context, for example whether the ‘right’ target is available and an absence of obstructions. However, rather than distinct factors, they are entangled, such that a maladapted brain, motivation for sexual domination, and a situation providing an opportunity for sexual killing can be reverse engineered back from murder(s). An excellent aspect of the book is that factors are explained individually as well as linked together thus increasing the depth of understanding, including using biographical sketches. The biographical sketches of eighty sexual serial killers seemed an overwhelming prospect to me, but it was truly invaluable. For any Open University students who have studied DE310 and/or read Mad or Bad? A Critical Approach to Counselling and Forensic Psychology (2017), there are elements which link between the two. For example, the snapshot of ‘Omar’ (p.210) and ‘Mr Bradford’ (pp.221-222), in Mad or Bad… (2017) can be seen in Understanding… in that these sketches involve contradictions and incongruent characteristics if comparing them, yet they are the same person. This is a critical aspect for understanding this population because so often, especially in the media, serial killers can be portrayed as non-human or otherwise an exception to ‘normal’ others. The biographical sketches are illuminating. They do contain difficult themes and experiences, but the book maintains a respectful tone in that it neither dismisses the crimes nor uses previous trauma as justification for them. No black-and-white conceptualisations of the grey. This respect for the lives stolen across the divide of killer and victim is further testament to the quality of the writing and expertise of Fred and Olga by including the nuances and making you reflect upon assumptions or opinions. The final word is the last section of the book. It closes the book and draws together the previous sections. The aim of building and reinforcing on the existing work by using psychological and neuroscientific theories and literature is well achieved. Understanding Sexual Serial Killing is excellent. It is a difficult subject and there are details which cause a visceral response. There are heart-breaking details among the facts. Fred and Olga acknowledge the tragedy and missed opportunities in which interventions could have led to different outcomes. There are biographies of names we recognise from the saturation of true crime media but stories we may not. There are unmistakably some difficult themes and experiences, but it is an accessible book which includes footnotes for additional information or references to the extensive research. Understanding Sexual Serial Killing is an immersive reading experience. The veracity of the information can deliver shocks and distress. I decided to not read at home. Reading the book in public, such as the park, was an interesting experience as details could hit differently. For example, when feeling afraid and hypervigilant one evening, I wondered if I might need to use the sizable book to defend myself. It can be tough to learn the depravity of some parents and the sequence of events behind murders: the split-second decisions or actions which ended in tragedy. Despite the formidable nature of the book, its accessibility allows for readers to put it down if needed to take a break, but it is so compelling you will be right back to it! Whether binge reading or snack reading, you can follow the science, themes, and narrative. This is vital. People from multiple backgrounds can benefit from reading the Understanding Sexual Serial Killing because the lessons and knowledge within the text can help in settings of educational, forensic, health, mental health, and beyond. For example, in the Preface, there is a line which stuck with me during and since reading the book: “in almost all cases, if not every case, the killers feel a deep resentment about things that happened to them, usually in childhood or adolescence” (p.xviii). Therefore, the more people who read this book, the more hope there may be to prevent future sexual serial killing. This is not a Pollyanna fantasy. The number of missed opportunities for alternative outcomes suggest that; far from being a deterministic, linear relationship between maladapted brains, motivations of dominance, and contexts of coalescing opportunity, the pattern can be broken but lessons must be learned. Understanding Sexual Serial Killing, by Frederick Toates and Olga Coschug-Toates is a must-read.
by Clara Clein Wolfe 01 Jan, 2021
Social Robots with Professor Emily Cross: From Social Cognition To Social Robots.
by Clara Clein Wolfe 04 Sept, 2020
Covid-19 (C19) continues to dominate 2020. Fluctuating rates of infection, devastating loss of life, widespread uncertainty, March lockdown, and more recent, local lockdowns, demonstrate the scale of the pandemic's impact upon societies. Psychological research has adapted to the constraints of the 'new normal' and remains studying the thoughts, feelings and behaviour that people exhibit.  On 22nd August 2020, the London branch of the Open University Psychological Society held an online conference entitled, Social Psychology Today. Dr Emma O'Dwyer, Professor John Drury and Professor Elizabeth Stokoe presented lectures about C19 Mutual Aid Groups, the role of social psychology in C19 responses, and the science of coronavirus.
by Clara Clein Wolfe 20 Jul, 2020
"In mindfulness, you cannot lose". Mindful Stress Management: Coming To Our 5 Senses.
More posts
Share by: